Dictionary Definition
empathy n : understanding and entering into
another's feelings
User Contributed Dictionary
English
Etymology
A twentieth-century borrowing of Ancient Greek ἐμπάθεια (empatheia), literally ‘passion’ (formed from εν- en-, ‘in, at’ + πάθος pathos ‘feeling’), coined by Rudolf Lotze to translate German Einfühlung. The modern Greek word εμπάθεια has an opposite meaning denoting strong negative feelings and prejudice against someone.Noun
- the intellectual identification of the thoughts, feelings, or state of another person
- capacity to understand another person's point of view or the result of such understanding
Translations
capacity to understand another person's point of
view
- Russian: умение поставить себя на место другого
Extensive Definition
Empathy is the capacity to recognise or
understand another's state of mind or emotion. It is often
characterized as the ability to "put oneself into another's shoes",
or to in some way experience the outlook or emotions of another
being within oneself. It may be described metaphorically as an
emotional kind of resonance or mirroring.
Etymology
The English word empathy is derived the Greek ἐμπάθεια (empatheia), "physical affection, passion, partiality" and that from ἐν (en), "in, at" + πάθος (pathos), "feeling". The term was adapted by Theodore Lipps to create the German word Einfühlung ("feeling into") from which the English term is then more directly derived.Discussion
Since empathy involves understanding the emotions of other people, the way it is characterised is derivative of the way emotions themselves are characterised. If for example, emotions are taken to be centrally characterised by bodily feelings, then grasping the bodily feelings of another will be central to empathy. On the other hand, if emotions are more centrally characterised by combinations of beliefs and desires, then grasping these beliefs and desires will be more essential to empathy.Furthermore, a distinction should be made between
deliberately imagining being another person, or being in their
situation, and simply recognizing their emotion. The ability to
imagine oneself as another person is a sophisticated imaginative
process. However the basic capacity to recognize emotions is
probably innate and may
be achieved unconsciously. Yet it can be trained, and achieved with
various degrees of intensity or accuracy.
The human capacity to recognize the bodily
feelings of another is related to one's imitative capacities, and
seems to be grounded in the innate capacity to associate the bodily
movements and facial expressions one sees in another with the
proprioceptive
feelings of producing those corresponding movements or expressions
oneself. Humans also seem to make the same immediate connection
between the tone of voice and other vocal expressions and inner
feeling. See neurological
basis below. There is some debate concerning how exactly the
conscious experience (or phenomenology) of empathy should be
characterized. The basic idea is that by looking at the facial
expressions or bodily movements of another, or by hearing their
tone of voice, one may get an immediate sense of how they feel (as
opposed to more intellectually noting the behavioral symptoms of
their emotion). Though empathic recognition is likely to involve
some form of arousal in the empathiser, they may not experience
this feeling as belonging to their own body, but instead likely to
perceptually locate the feeling 'in' the body of the other person.
Alternatively the empathiser may instead get a sense of an
emotional 'atmosphere' or that the emotion belongs equally to all
the parties involved.
More fully developed empathy requires more than
simply recognizing another's emotional state. Since emotions are
typically directed towards objects or states of affairs, the
empathiser may first require some idea of what that object might be
(where object can include imaginary objects, concepts, other
people, or even the empathiser). Alternatively the recognition of
the feeling may precede the recognition of the object of that
emotion, or even aid the empathiser in discovering the object of
the other's emotion. The empathiser may also need to determine how
the emotional state affects the way in which the other perceives
the object. For example, the empathizer needs to determine which
aspects of the object to focus on. Hence it is often not enough
that the empathiser recognize the object toward which the other is
directed, plus the bodily feeling, and then simply add these
components together. Rather the empathiser needs to find the way
into the loop where perception of the object affects feeling and
feeling affects the perception of the object. The following
sequence of examples identifies some of the major factors in
empathising with another:
I sense that:
- Frank is feeling annoyed, (via facial, vocal or postural expression).
- Frank is feeling annoyed due to not getting what he wants, (general object of emotion).
- Frank is feeling annoyed because he missed his train, (particular object of emotion)
- Frank is feeling annoyed because he missed his train, but only by a few seconds, (focus of particular object).
- Frank is feeling annoyed because he only just missed his train and he had an important meeting to get to, (background non-psychological context).
- Frank is feeling annoyed because he only just missed his train, and he had an important meeting and because he is generally an irritable sort of person (character traits).
Contrasting empathy with other phenomena
Empathy is distinct from sympathy, pity, emotional contagion, and telepathy. Sympathy is the feeling of compassion for another, the wish to see them better off or happier, often described as "feeling sorry" for someone. Pity is feeling that another is in trouble and in need of help as they cannot fix their problems themselves. Emotional contagion is when a person (especially an infant or a member of a mob) imitatively 'catches' the emotions that others are showing without necessarily recognising this is happening (Hatfield et al 1994). Telepathy is not a psychological phenomenon, but a supposedly paranormal phenomenon, whereby emotions or other mental states can be read directly, without needing to infer, or perceive expressive clues about the other person.Pity is, "Things are bad for you, you seem as
though you need help." Sympathy is, "I'm sorry for your sadness, I
wish to help." Emotional Contagion is, "You feel sad and now I feel
sad." Empathy is, "I recognise how you feel." Apathy is, "I don't
care how you feel. " Telepathy is, "I read your sadness without you
expressing it to me in any normal way."
Empathic development
By the age of 2, children normally begin to display the fundamental behaviors of empathy by having an emotional response that corresponds with another person. Even earlier, at one year of age, infants have some rudiments of empathy, in the sense that they understand that, just like their own actions, other people's actions have goals.. Sometimes, toddlers will comfort others or show concern for them as early as 24 months of age. Also during the second year, toddlers will play games of falsehood or "pretend" in an effort to fool others, and this requires that the child know what others believe before he or she can manipulate those beliefs (Feldman, 1997).In 1997, Douglas Olsen
defined empathetic maturity as the cognitive structure that
determines whether a person can feel or not feel empathy, who one
feels it for and how broad a group. Differences in empathetic
maturity are differences in the way a person relates self-created
meaning to meaning perceived in others. Empathetic maturity
provides the criteria for determining whether another will be
experienced as "like me" or "different." More inclusive criteria
increase the number and diversity of others who will be perceived
empathetically. The highest of the hierarchical stages of
empathetic maturity is the most inclusive where all others are
perceived as "like me." (Olsen, 2001) There are three stages of
empathetic maturity
(Olsen, 2001; and
Olsen, 1997):
Stage 1 – This most primitive pattern and not
common in adults. Persons at this stage see others as fundamentally
different from themselves. The rationales for another's actions,
feelings, or thoughts are not experienced as having human relevance
in the sense that one’s own rationales do. Those operating at this
stage perceive mutuality with others concretely.
Stage 2 – People at Stage 2 hold that their
rationales for behavior are valid for everyone. And so, reasons for
behaviors and feelings are legitimate to the degree they coincide
with the person at Stage 2. Unlike Stage 1, the Stage 2 person sees
others like him or her so long as they make sense of their world
the same way. Therefore, positive regard for a sufferer perceived
to be participating in negative behaviors is difficult for the
Stage 2 person unless the behavior is explicable from his or her
point of view. An example of such negative behavior would be AIDS
as the result of sex practices not condoned by the Stage 2
observer. If the Stage 2 person believes the sufferer is
responsible for the behavior, he or she will have no empathy. If
the Stage 2 person can detect an acceptable reason why the sufferer
is not actually responsible, for example, illness resulted from
blood tranfusion, beyond the sufferer's control, then empathy
emerges. [Note: This "example" confuses empathy per se, being the
ability to recreate in one's mind the emotional or cognitive state
of mind of another being and so understand that other being, with
the possible resulting sympathy/compassion a person feels towards a
sufferer as a result of the empathy. Whether sympathy/compassion
occurs clearly also depends on the empath's value judgments and
understanding of what caused the suffering, but the empathy that
allows the person to understand that suffering occurs is still
present.] Caregivers at Stage 2 who want to feel empathetic toward
their patients often try to find factors that mitigate
responsibility. Most of society operates at Stage 2.
Stage 3 – At this stage, mutuality occurs prior
to any judgment about the person's behavior. The other is perceived
as human in the same way the self is experienced, based solely on
being a creator of meaning rather than on the content of the
meanings created. The perception of another person as responsible
for a problem no longer has the power to hinder the development of
empathy. If the sufferer is seen as responsible, there is no longer
any need to mitigate that responsibility as a method for allowing
empathy. A hallmark of Stage 3 is a person's ability to perceive
another empathetically while simultaneously and without apparent
contradiction perceiving that other as responsible for problematic
behavior.
Neurological basis
Research in recent years has focused on possible brain processes as concomitant with empathy (Preston & de Waal, 2002). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has recently been employed to investigate the functional anatomy of empathy (for reviews see Decety & Jackson, 2006; Decety & Lamm, 2006; deVignemont & Singer, 2006). These studies have shown that observing another person’s emotional state activates parts of the neuronal network involved in processing that same state in oneself, whether it is disgust (Wicker et al., 2003), touch (Keysers et al., 2004), or pain (Morrison et al., 2004; Jackson et al., 2005, 2006; Lamm et al., 2007; Singer et al., 2004, 2006).The study of empathic neuronal circuitries was
inspired by the discovery of mirror
neurons in monkeys that fire both when the creature watches
another perform an action as well as when they themselves perform
it presents a possible neural mechanism for mapping others'
feelings onto one's own nervous system. Based on some of this work,
the
simulation theory of empathy has been developed.
Lack of empathy
Some psychologists, psychiatrists, and other scientists believe that not all humans have an ability to feel empathy or perceive the emotions of others. For instance, Autism and related conditions such as Asperger's syndrome are often (but not always) characterized by an apparent reduced ability to empathize with others. The interaction between empathy and autism spectrum disorders is a complex and ongoing field of research, and is discussed in detail below.According to recent fMRI studies the syndrome of
alexithymia, a
condition in which an individual is rendered incapable of
recognising and articulating emotional arousal in self or others,
is responsible for a severe lack of emotional empathy. The lack of
empathetic attunement inherent to alexithymic states may reduce
quality and satisfaction of relationships.
According to Simon
Baron-Cohen's ideas, an absence of empathy might also be
related to an absence of theory of
mind (i.e., the ability to model
another's world view using either a theory-like analogy between
oneself and others, or the ability to simulate pretend mental
states and then apply the consequences of these simulations to
others). Again with regard to autism, not all autistics fit this
pattern, and the theory remains controversial, and does not
differentiate between cognitive empathy and affective empathy, nor
do autistic people lack compassion. Francesca Happe
showed that autistic children who demonstrate a lack of theory of
mind (cognitive empathy) lack theory if mind for self as well as
others .
In contrast, psychopaths are seemingly
able to demonstrate the appearance of sensing the emotions of
others with such a theory of mind, often demonstrating care and
friendship in a convincing manner, and can use this ability to
charm or manipulate, but they crucially lack the sympathy or
compassion that empathy often leads to. However, it has been
claimed that components of circuitry involved in empathy may also
be dysfunctional in psychopathy (Tunstall N., Fahy T. and McGuire
P. in: Guide to Neuroimaging in Psychiatry, Eds. Fu C et al, Martin
Dunitz: London 2003). Empathy certainly does not guarantee
benevolence. The same ability may underlie schadenfreude (taking
pleasure in the pain of another entity) and sadism
(being sexually gratified through the infliction of pain or
humiliation on another person).
Empathy and autism spectrum disorders
A common source of confusion in analyzing the interactions between empathy and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) is that the apparent lack of empathy may mask at least two other underlying causes:- Excessive sensitivity or "overwhelm," may be a cause of early learned suppression.
- Failure to demonstrate empathy can arise from inability (or not knowing how) to express empathy to others, as opposed to difficulty feeling it, internally.
With these possible exceptions noted, one would
be grossly misled to infer that individuals with ASD's are
generally exceptional empathizers when, quite to the contrary,
research indicates the reverse. Research suggests that many ASD
individuals have a lack of theory of
mind (ToM) and alexithymia (85% of those
with ASD's have alexithymia), both of which conditions involve
severe deficits in the individual's ability to be empathetically
attuned to others. Alexithymia
involves not just the inability to verbally express emotions, but
specifically the inability to identify emotional states in self or
others. However, research by Rogers et al. suggests that empathy
needs to be differentiated between cognitive empathy and affective
empathy in people with Asperger syndrome, suggesting autistic
individuals have less developed understanding of the feeling of
others, but demonstrate equally much empathy when aware of others
state of mind, and respond more to stress experienced by other
people than non-autistic people.
Dapretto et al. (2006) found that, relative to
typically developing children, high-functioning children with
autism showed reduced mirror neuron activity in the brain's
inferior frontal
gyrus (pars opercularis) while imitating and observing
emotional expressions. The authors suggest this supports the
hypothesis that a dysfunctional mirror neuron system may underlie
the social deficits observed in autism.
Practical issues
Empathic methods
Proper empathetic engagement is supposed to help to understand and anticipate the behavior of the other. Apart from the automatic tendency to recognise the emotions of others, one may also deliberately engage in empathic reasoning. Two general methods have been identified here (e.g. Goldie 2000).- simulate 'pretend' versions of the beliefs, desires, character traits and context of the other and see what emotional feelings this leads to;
- simulate the emotional feeling and then look around for a suitable reason for this to fit.
Empathic accuracy
Some research suggests that people are more able and willing to empathize with those most similar to themselves. In particular, empathy increases with similarities in culture and living conditions. We are also more likely to empathize with those with whom we interact more frequently (See Levenson and Reuf 1997 and Hoffman 2000: 62).A measure of how well a person can infer the
specific content of another person's thoughts and feelings has been
developed by William Ickes (1997, 2003). Ickes and his colleagues
have developed a video-based method to measure empathic accuracy
and have used this method to study the empathic inaccuracy of
maritally aggressive and abusive husbands, among other
topics.
In general, there are concerns that the
empathiser's own emotional background may affect or distort what
emotions they perceive in others (e.g. Goleman 1996: p. 104).
Empathy is not a process that is likely to deliver certain
judgements about the emotional states of others. It is a skill that
is gradually developed throughout life, and which improves the more
contact we have with the person with whom we empathise.
Accordingly, any knowledge we gain of the emotions of the other
must be revisable in light of further information.
Ethical issues
It should be noted that the extent to which a person's emotions are publicly observable, or mutually recognised as such has significant social conseqences. Empathic recognition may or may not be welcomed or socially desirable. This is particularly the case where we recognise the emotions that someone has towards ourselves during real time interactions. Based on a metaphorical affinity with touch, Philosopher Edith Wyschogrod claims that the proximity entailed by empathy increases the potential vulnerability of either party.The appropriate role of empathy in our dealings
with others is highly dependent on the circumstances. For instance,
it is claimed that clinicians or caregivers must take care not to
be too sensitive to the emotions of others, to over-invest their
own emotions, at the risk of draining away their own
resourcefulness. Furthmore an awareness of the limitations of
empathic accuracy is prudent in a caregiving situation.
Intense Empathy
In addition to the above use, the term empathy is also used by some people to signify their heightened or higher sensitivity to the emotions and state of others. Empathy may be here conceptualised as the ability to fully "read" another person, completely translating each movement into understandable conversation. This, reportedly, can lead to both positive aspects such as a more skilled instinct for what is "behind the scenes" with people, but also to difficulties such as rapid over-stimulation, or overwhelming stress caused by an inability to protect oneself from this so-called 'pick-up'. Such people may for example find crowds stressful simply due to picking up what is often described as "white noise" or multiple emotions as they pass through it, a phenomenon not to be confused with agoraphobia and sometimes informally known as crowd-sickness. A recurrent theme of discussion on such websites relates to the impact upon individuals, and therefore also methods (including mental practices, emotional processes and ritual) which anecdotally can help reduce the intensity of empathic reactions to others' feelings to a more bearable level (informally called 'shielding' or emotional detachment).Empathy in this sense is ascribed by such people
to various mechanisms. These include simply more sophisticated
subconscious processing of sensory cues or stronger emotional
feedback than the norm,
(i.e. the normal human experience but more so), and therefore fit
within present models.
Some people, perhaps due to synesthesia, believe it
instead to be a direct emotional sense or a feel for others'
"energy". The New Age
religion(s) have constructed belief systems around anecdotal
evidence of persons who claim to be empaths in this sense. This
aspect of empathy is not clinically recognized, and someone calling
themselves an "empath" usually does not intend to imply that they
are gifted with any psychic
ability.
In general empathy may be painful to oneself:
seeing the pain of others, especially as broadcasted by mass media,
can cause one temporary or permanent clinical
depression; a phenomenon which is sometimes called weltschmerz. However, since
a basic emotional understanding of others is an important
pre-requisite of human relationships, subjects face a dilemma to
protect oneself from the pain of empathy or seek to relate to other
humans despite the potential risk of injury.
Disciplinary approaches to empathy
Empathy and psychotherapy
Heinz Kohut is the main introducer of the principle of empathy in psychoanalysis. His principle applies to the method of gathering unconscious material. The possibility of not applying the principle is granted in the cure. For instance when you must reckon with another principle, that of reality.Developing skills of empathy is often a central
theme in the recovery process for drug addicts.
Empathy and evolutionary psychology
In evolutionary psychology, attempts at explaining pro-social behavior often mention the presence of empathy in the individual as a possible variable. Although exact motives behind complex social behaviors are difficult to distinguish, the "ability to put oneself in the shoes of another person and experience events and emotions the way that person experienced them" is the definitive factor for truly altruistic behavior according to Batson's empathy-altruism hypothesis. If empathy is not felt, social exchange (what's in it for me?) supersedes pure altruism, but if empathy is felt, an individual will help regardless of whether it is in their self-interest to do so and even if the costs outweigh potential rewards.Empathy and education
An important target by the method Learning by teaching (LdL) is to train systematically and in each lesson the students-empathy. They have to transmit new contents to the classmates, so they have to reflect continuously on the mental processes by the other students in the classroom. This way - in addition - it is possible to develop step by step the students-feeling for group-reactions and networking.Empathy and animals
Some students of animal behavior claim that empathy is not restricted to humans as the definition implies. Examples include dolphins saving humans (sympathy) from drowning or from shark attacks, and a multitude of behaviors observed in primates, both in captivity and in the wild. See, for instance, the popular book The Ape and the Sushi Master by Frans de Waal. Rodents have been shown to demonstrate empathy for cagemates (but not strangers) in pain.Furthermore people can empathize with animals. As
such, empathy is thought to be a driving psychological force behind
the animal
rights movement (an example of sympathy), whether or not using
empathy is justified by any real similarity between the emotional
experiences of animals and humans.
Empathy and fiction
Some philosophers (such as Martha Nussbaum) suggest that novel reading cultivates readers' empathy and leads them to exercise better world citizenship. For a critique of this application of the empathy-altruism hypothesis to experiences of narrative empathy, see Keen's Empathy and the Novel (Oxford, 2007).In some works of science
fiction and fantasy,
empathy is understood to be a paranormal or psychic
ability to sense the emotions of others, as opposed to telepathy, which allows one to
perceive thoughts as well. A person who has that ability is also
called an "empath" or "telempath" in this context. Occasionally
these empaths are also able to project their own emotions, or to
affect the emotions of others.
Empathy and history
Some Post-Modern historians such as Keith Jenkins in recent years have debated whether or not it is possible to empathise with people from the past. Jenkins argues that empathy only enjoys such a privileged position in the present because it corresponds harmoniously with the dominant Liberal discourse of modern society and can be connected to John Stuart Mill's concept of reciprocal freedom. Jenkins argues the past is a foreign country and as we do not have access to the epistemological conditions of bygone ages we are unable to empathise. The reader will not be astonished at the conclusive issue about empathy and history. Only events and their products meet or not empathy. It is impossible to forecast the effect of empathy on the future. We can pay attention to the means of language of telling events. We above checked a contemporary subject may not take part in the past. A past subject may take part in the present by the so-called historic present. If we watch from a fictitious past, can tell the present with the future tense, as it happens with the trick of the false prophecy. There is no way of telling the present with the means of the past. The way of making the study of empathy functional is still long.Empathy and moral theory
In research published in 2007 in the book "The Ethics of Care and Empathy," Philosopher Michael Slote introduces a theory of care based ethics that is grounded in empathy. His claim is that moral motivation does, and should stem from a basis of empathic response. He claims that our natural reaction to situations of moral significance are explained by empathy. He explains that the limits and obligations of empathy and in turn morality are natural. These natural obligations include a greater empathic, and moral obligation to family and friends, along with an account of temporal and physical distance. In situations of close temporal and physical distance, and with family or friends, our moral obligation seems stronger to us than with strangers at a distance naturally. Slote explains that this is due to empathy and our natural empathic ties. He further adds that actions are wrong if and only if they reflect or exhibit a deficiency of fully developed empathic concern for others on the part of the agent.Empathy and music
The metaphor of musical resonance reinforces certain ideas around empathy that are often misconstrued. Gauss, suggests that, “In popular usage the idea refers to the emotional resonance between two people, when, like strings tuned to the same frequency, each responds in perfect sympathy to the other and each reinforces the responses of the other”However, within the musical semantic universe,
the better metaphor is that of overtones and undertones, by which
an instrument incapable of replicating a particular frequency
(pitch) a nevertheless resonate with pitches sharing certain
harmonic structures. Harmonic resonance, unlike pitch replication,
suggests appropriate differentiation between the two instruments,
between model and beholder, while retaining a sense that some
accuracy is required.
Interestingly, one Chinese translation for
empathy contains the two characters not for the replication of
pitch, but for harmonic resonance. This Chinese translation aligns
with the forms of empathy which arise intuitively or
non-cognitively.
Notable theorists of empathy
- Edith Stein: Empathy… is the experience of foreign consciousness in general
- Heinz Kohut: Empathy is the capacity to think and feel oneself into the inner life of another person.
- Nancy Eisenberg: An affective response that stems from the apprehension or comprehension of another’s emotional state or condition, and that is similar to what the other person is feeling or would be expected to feel
- Roy Schafer: Empathy involves the inner experience of sharing in and comprehending the momentary psychological state of another person.
- D. M. Berger: The capacity to know emotionally what another is experiencing from within the frame of reference of that other person, the capacity to sample the feelings of another or to put oneself in another’s shoes.
- R. R. Greenson: To empathize means to share, to experience the feelings of another person.
- Wynn Schwartz: ''"We recognize others as empathic when we feel that they have accurately acted on or somehow acknowledged in stated or unstated fashion our values or motivations, our knowledge, and our skills or competence, but especially as they appear to recognize the significance of our actions in a manner that we can tolerate their being recognized."
- Carl Rogers: To perceive the internal frame of reference of another with accuracy and with the emotional components and meanings which pertain thereto as if one were the person, but without ever losing the "as if" condition. Thus, it means to sense the hurt or the pleasure of another as he senses it and to perceive the causes thereof as he perceives them, but without ever losing the recognition that it is as if I were hurt or pleased and so forth.
- Jean Decety: a sense of similarity in feelings experienced by the self and the other, without confusion between the two individuals.
- Martin Hoffman: An affective response more appropriate to another's situation than one's own.''
See also
- Affect display
- Charisma
- Compersion
- Introjection
- Psychology
- Antisocial personality disorder
- Folk psychology
- Theory of mind
- Emotional intelligence
- Emotional intelligence tests
- Emotional competence
- Intercultural competence
- John D. Mayer
- Marshall Rosenberg
- Peter Salovey
- Daniel Goleman
- Machiavellian intelligence
- Spatial empathy
References
Bibliography
- Austin, J. L. (1979). 'Other Minds'. In Philosophical Papers. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 76-116.
- Batson, C. D., Håkansson Eklund, J., Chermok, V. L., Hoyt, J. L., & Ortiz, B. G. (2007). An additional antecedent of empathic concern: Valuing the welfare of the person in need. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), 65-74.
- B. Bower "Goal-oriented brain cells: neurons may track action as a prelude to empathy" in Science News, April 30, 2005
- Corazza, Eros (2004). "Empathy, Imagination, and Reports". Chapter 7 in Reflecting the Mind - Indexicality and Quasi-Indexicality. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
- Currie, Greg (2004). "Anne Brontë and the uses of imagination". Chapter 9 in Arts and Minds. Oxford University Press, Oxford.
- Currie, Greg & Ravenscroft, Ian (2002). Recreative Minds: Imagination in Philosophy and Psychology. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
- Dapretto, M. et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28-30.
- Davis, Mark H. (1996). Empathy: A Social-Psychological Approach. Westview.
- Decety, J., & Jackson, P.L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71-100.
- Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. The Scientific World Journal, 6, 1146–1163.
- Feldman, R.S. (1997). Development across the life span. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
- Goldie, Peter (2000). The Emotions, A Philosophical Exploration. Clarendon Press, Oxford.
- Håkansson, J., & Montgomery, H. (2003). Empathy as an interpersonal phenomenon. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 20(3), 267-284.
- Hoffman, M. L. (1978), "Empathy, Its Development and Prosocial Implications", in C. B. Keasey (ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 25: 169-218.
- Hoffman, M. L. (2000), Empathy and Moral Development (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
- Ickes, W. (Ed.) (1997). Empathic Accuracy. Guilford Press, New York.
- Ickes, W. (2003). Everyday Mind Reading: Understanding What Other People Think and Feel. Prometheus Books.
- Jackson, P.L., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A.N., & Decety, J. (2006). Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain: An event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 44, 752-61.
- Keen, Suzanne (2007). Empathy and the Novel. Oxford University Press.
- Lamm, C., Batson, C.D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural basis of human empathy – Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 42-58.
- Lampert, K.(2005); Traditions of Compassion: From Religious Duty to Social Activism. Palgrave-Macmillan; ISBN 1-4039-8527-8
- Levenson, R. W. and Reuf, A. M. (1997), "Physiological Aspects of Emotional Knowledge and Rapport", in W. Ickes (Ed.), Empathic Accuracy (New York: Guilford), 44-72.
- Rose Rosetree (2001), "Empowered by Empathy". Women's Intuition Worldwide, ISBN 0-9651145-8-9.
- Stein, Edith (1917), "On the problem of empathy". ICS Publications, Washington, 1989, ISBN 0-935216-11-1
- Evan Thompson (ed.)(2001), "Between Ourselves. Second-Person Issues in the Study of Consciousness", Imprint Academic, 2001 ISBN 0 907845 14 2; Journal of Consciousness studies, 8, number 5-7, 2001 ISSN 1355 8250
- Slote, Michael (2007). "The Ethics of Care and Empathy."
External links
- Evolutionary Aspects of Love and Empathy
- The Joys and Pitfalls of Being an Empath
- News about empathy
- Dictionary of the History of Ideas: Empathy
- Empathic listening skills How to listen so others will feel heard, or listening first aid (University of California). Download a one hour seminar on empathic listening and attending skills.
- Literature about empathy Articles, books, and book chapters about empathy
- Empathy as a basic brain function (New Scientist, 2004)
- IQ is Only Half The Picture: Cultivating your Child's Emotional Intelligence
- To hear a definition of empathy given by Marshall Rosenberg (Nonviolent communication), through a parallel between empathy and surf.
- The Empathy Symbol Just like the peace symbol, there is now an empathy symbol.
- Exploring the phenomenon of empathy Doctoral Dissertation
- Empathize Empathize promotes the use of cooperative learning in schools as well as the building of schools in Cyprus and Jerusalem for the children of opposing governments.
- The Paradox of Empathy Discussion about whether it is possible to empathize with another.
- The Swedish Empathy Center Organizes knowledge about empathy across disciplines
- Greater Good magazine article examines human empathy Articles about empathy
- Study: People Literally Feel Pain of Others - mirror-touch synesthesia Live Science, 17 June 2007
empathy in Bulgarian: Емпатия
empathy in Czech: Empatie
empathy in Danish: Empati
empathy in German: Empathie
empathy in Estonian: Empaatia
empathy in Spanish: Empatía
empathy in Basque: Enpatia
empathy in French: Empathie
empathy in Korean: 감정이입
empathy in Indonesian: Empati
empathy in Italian: Empatia
empathy in Hebrew: אמפתיה
empathy in Lithuanian: Empatija
empathy in Hungarian: Empátia
empathy in Dutch: Empathie
empathy in Japanese: 共感
empathy in Norwegian: Empati
empathy in Norwegian Nynorsk: Empati
empathy in Polish: Empatia
empathy in Portuguese: Empatia
empathy in Russian: Эмпатия
empathy in Slovenian: Empatija
empathy in Serbian: Емпатија
empathy in Finnish: Empatia
empathy in Swedish: Empati
empathy in Turkish: Empati (psikoloji)
empathy in Ukrainian: Емпатія
empathy in Chinese: 同理心
empathy in Slovak: Vcítenie
Synonyms, Antonyms and Related Words
accent,
accentuation,
accord, accordance, affinity, agape, agreement, allergy, amity, anaphylaxis, appreciation, attention, bonds of harmony,
brotherly love, caring,
caritas, cement of
friendship, charity,
chord, communion, community, community of
interests, compassion, compatibility, comprehension, concern, concord, concordance, congeniality, considerateness,
correspondence,
delicacy, echo, emphasis, esprit, esprit de corps, exquisiteness, feeling of
identity, fellow feeling, fellowship, fineness, force, frictionlessness, good
vibes, good vibrations, happy family, harmony, hyperesthesia, hyperpathia, hypersensitivity,
identification,
identity, insistence, involvement, irritability, kinship, like-mindedness,
love, mutuality, nervousness, oneness, oversensibility,
oversensitiveness,
overtenderness,
passibility,
pathos, peace, perceptiveness, perceptivity, photophobia, prickliness, rapport, rapprochement, reciprocity, relating, response, responsiveness, sensitiveness, sensitivity, sensitization, sharing, solidarity, soreness, stress, supersensitivity,
sympathetic chord, sympathetic response, sympathy, symphony, tact, tactfulness, team spirit,
tenderness, tetchiness, thin skin,
ticklishness,
touchiness, understanding, union, unison, unity, vibes, vibrations, warmth, weight